Research Article |
Corresponding author: Amanda Ayala ( apayala@ecosur.edu.mx ) Academic editor: Jose Fernandez-Triana
© 2018 Amanda Ayala, Gabriela Pérez-Lachaud, Jorge Toledo, Pablo Liedo, Pablo Montoya.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Ayala A, Pérez-Lachaud G, Toledo J, Liedo P, Montoya P (2018) Host acceptance by three native braconid parasitoid species attacking larvae of the Mexican fruit fly, Anastrepha ludens (Diptera, Tephritidae). Journal of Hymenoptera Research 63: 33-49. https://doi.org/10.3897/jhr.63.23724
|
We studied the oviposition and host acceptance behavior of three braconid parasitoid species native to Mexico, Doryctobracon crawfordi (Viereck), Opius hirtus (Fischer), and Utetes anastrephae (Viereck), with potential to be considered as biocontrol agents against tephritid fruit fly pests in the Neotropics. Third instar larvae of Anastrepha ludens (Loew), with and without previous parasitization by conspecifics, were simultaneously offered to females of each species, and the individual behavior was video recorded to construct oviposition flow diagrams. The patterns of foraging and host acceptance were similar in the studied species; all rejected mostly parasitized hosts suggesting that this strategy is common in the guild of larval parasitoids attacking Anastrepha spp. The complete searching and host acceptance process took 2.2 ± 0.1 min (mean ± SE) in D. crawfordi, 1.7 ± 0.1 s in U. anastrephae and 1.5 ± 0.1 s in O. hirtus. Notably, because of toxins injected by parasitoid females during oviposition, the parasitized hosts experienced a transient paralysis of variable duration. Hosts attacked by U. anastrephae remained immobile for the shortest time (12.5 ± 1 min) (mean±SE), followed by D. crawfordi (20.5 ± 3.4 min) and O. hirtus (24.1 ± 2 min). Our data revealed a notable discrimination ability in all three species, and that behavioral differences lay mainly in the time of parasitization and in the duration of paralysis experienced by attacked hosts. This suggest that the three species could be valuable as biocontrol agents, but additional studies are necessary to better understand the advantages and limitations of each one as natural enemies of fruit fly pests.
Host discrimination, transient host paralysis, biocontrol agents, Doryctobracon crawfordi , Opius hirtus , Utetes anastrephae
Fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae) are considered one of the main fruit pests worldwide (
Parasitoids are insects whose larvae develop by feeding in or on the body of other arthropods, usually insects; larval feeding almost always results in the death of the host (
Once female parasitoids have located their hosts, they have the capacity to distinguish between parasitized and not parasitized hosts, a strategy known as discrimination ability (
Host location and host acceptance behavior has been widely studied in the generalist fruit fly parasitoid Diachasmimorpha longicaudata (Ashmead) (
Doryctobracon crawfordi is native in habitats above 600 masl from Mexico to Argentina (
The purpose of this study was to compare the foraging and host acceptance behaviors of the parasitoid species D. crawfordi, U. anastrephae and O. hirtus on previously parasitized and non-parasitized larvae of A. ludens, using video recording equipment under laboratory conditions. This knowledge should allow an improved understanding of the oviposition performance and potential of these parasitoid species as biocontrol agents against fruit fly pest species.
The experiments were conducted in the Biological Control laboratory of the Moscafrut Program SAGARPA-IICA, located in Metapa de Dominguez, Chiapas, Mexico. The parasitoid colonies were initiated from field infested fruits and maintained at 25±1 °C, 70±5% HR with a photoperiod of 12:12 (L:D) h. Eight-day-old larvae of A. ludens mixed with artificial diet were provided as host by the Moscafrut facility, where this species is mass reared as described by
Parasitized host larvae were obtained by exposing groups of approximately 100 host larvae for two hours to 100 females and 50 males of each species separately. Larvae with three or more oviposition scars were considered as being successfully parasitized (
Copulated females, 5–6 day old with previous experience of oviposition were used. To gain this experience, groups of ~150 recently emerged adults (1female: 1male) were confined in aluminum frame acrylic cages (30 × 30 × 30 cm) and provided with water and honey as a source of food. Twenty-four hours before conducting the bioassays, ~200 A. ludens larvae mixed with larval diet were offered to these parasitoids in a Petri dish oviposition unit, for 2 h.
The host searching and acceptance performance of individual parasitoid females was observed with two different types of A. ludens host larvae that were exposed simultaneously: 1) larvae previously parasitized (24 h earlier) by conspecifics, and 2) larvae with no previous parasitization. Bioassays were conducted in oviposition units consisting of Petri dishes (55 mm in diameter × 9 mm in depth) with the edges reduced to five mm in depth and a central division of 5 mm to separate the two type of larvae. Five previously parasitized A. ludens larvae were placed in one of the two sides, and five non-parasitized larvae, of the same age, were placed in the other side. The oviposition unit was covered with an organza elastic cloth and secured with a rubber band in order to prevent larval escape. This cloth was semi-transparent making possible the observation of the host larvae through it. Guava juice was added on the surface of the cloth in order to attract the females and keep them on the parasitization units until larval detection.
The oviposition sequences of thirty females per species were observed and video recordings made with a Samsung KREUZNACH video camera (f = 2.3–78.2 mm; F:1.6; ø30.5). One female was released onto the surface of the oviposition unit in each observation. The larvae and females were replaced after each observation, as well as the cloth and the oviposition unit. Environmental conditions were 25 ± 1 °C and 75 ± 5% RH. Bioassays were conducted between 8:30 and 15:00 and the time of observation was ~1 h per female. If the female presented null activity for the first five minutes, it was replaced. Time of latency (defined here as “time that elapsed between two ovipositions”), the number of ovipositions, oviposition attempts, duration of oviposition and duration of host paralysis following oviposition (from the moment the stung larva remained immobile, to the moment it resumed crawling), were recorded for both host types. Video recordings were independently analyzed using the Movie Maker software version 2.6.4037.0, in order to obtain the sequences and transition frequencies of the different behaviors.
The number of ovipositions and oviposition attempts on the two larval types were compared using the t test for each parasitoid species. In order to compare the time spent on the different activities observed among the three species, a one-way analysis of variance with the Tukey-HSD test was conducted. Prior to analysis, a Box -Cox transformation of the data was conducted. For all analyses we used the JMP Starter software version 7.0.1 (
The general behavioral sequences of the three parasitoid species on the two host types were identified. The operational definitions for the observed behaviors are presented in Table
Definitions of the different behaviors exhibited by Utetes anastrephae, Doryctobracon crawfordi and Opius hirtus while foraging for host larvae.
Behavior | Description |
---|---|
1. Walking | Female walking on the oviposition unit surface, antennae not directed to the substrate |
2. Searching for a host | While walking the female touches the surface of the oviposition unit with the antennae |
3. Detection of a host | The female stays immobile over a host larva |
4. Oviposition attempt | Insertion of the ovipositor in order to have contact with the host. The latter is very mobile |
5. Oviposition | Oviposition, the female remains immobile during a certain period of time with the ovipositor inserted in the interior of the host larva |
6. Rejection | The female inserts the ovipositor in the host for a few seconds, but withdraws the ovipositor without actually laying an egg. |
7. Failure | When the female inserts the ovipositor in the oviposition unit without having contact with some host, mainly by the escape of the larvae |
In general, the females walked on the surface of the oviposition unit with their antennae in close contact with the surface of the oviposition unit. Once the females detected a larva, they attempted to establish contact with the host by introducing their ovipositor and began a movement of abdominal vibration (associated with the descent of the egg (
Ethogram of oviposition of females of Doryctobracon crawfordi on non-parasitized larvae (a) and larvae previously parasitized by conspecifics (b) under laboratory conditions. The width of the arrow is proportional to the relative frequency of transition. The numbers associated with the arrows represent the observed frequencies of the successive behaviors of a complex sequence of behavior (proportions are indicated in parentheses).
Ethogram of oviposition of females of Utetes anastrephae on non-parasitized larvae (a) and larvae previously parasitized by conspecifics (b) under laboratory conditions. The width of the arrow is proportional to the relative frequency of transition. The numbers associated with the arrows represent the observed frequencies of the successive behaviors of a complex sequence of behavior (proportions are indicated in parentheses).
Ethogram of oviposition of females of Opius hirtus on non-parasitized larvae (a) and larvae previously parasitized by conspecifics (b) under laboratory conditions. The width of the arrow is proportional to the relative frequency of transition. The numbers associated with the arrows represent the observed frequencies of the successive behaviors of a complex sequence of behavior (proportions are indicated in parentheses).
No marked differences in the flow diagrams were observed between non-parasitized hosts and parasitized hosts for any of the braconids studied here. However, females significantly rejected hosts previously parasitized by conspecifics following insertion of the ovipositor compared to those not parasitized (F = 2.35; df = 2, P < 0.001). Overall, U. anastrephae females rejected 79% of parasitized hosts, D. crawfordi 74% and O. hirtus 62%. Furthermore, a more intensive searching was observed when a failure (because the host moved away) occurred when attacking non-parasitized hosts than when attacking parasitized hosts. The complete process of searching and host acceptance (from the beginning of the observation until ovipositor removal) was completed in 2.2 ± 0.8 min (mean ± SE) in D. crawfordi, 1.7 ± 0.75 min in U. anastrephae and 1.52 ± 0.75 min in O. hirtus.
The time elapsed between ovipositions differed significantly between U. anastrephae and the other two species when the hosts had previously been parasitized (F = 0.5, df = 2, P < 0.05; N = 30). Regarding the time of latency with non-parasitized larvae, U. anastrephae presented the shortest time (3.25 ± 0.3 min) (mean ± SE) (Fig.
Latency (average ± SE, in minutes) between ovipositions of three native opine parasitoids attacking non-parasitized and previously parasitized Anastrepha ludens larvae. Different capital letters indicate statistically significant difference between the bars. Different letters, indicate statistically significant difference between the bars. Different lower case letters, indicate statistically significant difference between species.
The first host choice in the three parasitoid species corresponded mostly to the non-parasitized larvae (D. crawfordi 22/30; U. anastrephae 18/30 and O. hirtus 19/30). Utetes anastrephae parasitized a significantly (F = 3.39, df = 2, P = 0.03) higher quantity of non-parasitized hosts (3.3 ± 0.25) compared to the other two species (D. crawfordi 2.7 ± 0.23 and O. hirtus 2.3 ± 0.32). Doryctobracon crawfordi performed a greater number of oviposition attempts than U. anastrephae and O. hirtus in both types of larvae (Table
Average values (±SE) of number of ovipositions and attempts at oviposition on host larvae parasitized by conspecifics and non-parasitized host larvae.
Species of parasitoid | Number of ovipositions | Number of oviposition attempts (rejections) | N | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non-parasitized larvae | Parasitized larvae | Non-parasitized larvae | Parasitized larvae | ||
Doryctobracon crawfordi | 2.7±0.23ab | 0.6±0.15* | 56.9±5.1a | 42.4±7.5a | 30 |
Utetes anastrephae | 3.3±0.25a | 1.3±0.23* | 12.2±2.1b | 14.7±2.2b | 30 |
Opius hirtus | 2.3±0.32b | 1±0.16* | 15.9±2.2b | 12.26±2.3b | 30 |
The time of ovipositor insertion on previously parasitized larvae differed significantly (F = 4.7, df = 2, P = 0.001) among species, with D. crawfordi spending more time with the ovipositor inserted, and O. hirtus the shortest one (Table
Average values (±SE) of duration of oviposition, vibration of the abdomen of the females, and immobility of the host after stinging (all in minutes) in non-parasitized and parasitized host larvae of A. ludens.
Species | Duration of oviposition | Vibration of the abdomen | Host immobility | N | |||
Unparasitized host | Parasitized host | Unparasitized host | Parasitized host | Unparasitized host | Parasitized host | ||
Doryctobracon crawfordi | 2.2±0.1a | 2.2±0.1 | 0.35±0.01a | 0.35±0.03 | 21.3±1.2a | 20.5±3.4 | 30 |
Utetes anastrephae | 1.6±0.1b | 1.9±0.1 | 0.28±0.01ab | 0.26±0.01 | 13.4±0.6b | 12.1±1 | 30 |
Opius hirtus | 1.5±0.1b | 1.2±0.1* | 0.26±0.01b | 0.3±0.01 | 23.8±1.2a | 24.5±2 | 30 |
Knowledge on host acceptance behavior in insect parasitoids is fundamental to improve our understanding on the plant-herbivore-natural enemy tritrophic relations (
Several studies have indicated that responses of natural enemies are mediated mainly by chemical signals detected in the environment (
The three studied species presented typical behavior of antennal contact with the surface of the oviposition unit during the process of searching for the host larvae, which is an important step for host detection (
According to our results, the three parasitoid species have a high discrimination ability in the form defined by
Doryctobracon crawfordi presented the longest time spent on oviposition compared to the other two species. Host acceptance may depend on extrinsic factors such as host availability and quality, as well as intrinsic factors such as the quantity of eggs in the females, the age and their nutritional state (
Though koinobionts do not arrest host development, some species can induce transient host paralysis (temporary paralysis after being stung by the female wasp; e.g.
The factors associated with host immobility are toxic substances in a mixture such as venom, as well as polydnaviruses (PDVs) that function as regulatory elements and disrupt the host metabolism (
There are few studies regarding the oviposition behavior of opiine parasitoid species native to the Neotropical region, which makes our data of valuable importance. Our study reveals that behavioral differences among the studied parasitoid species lay mainly in the time of parasitization and in the time for which the parasitized hosts remained immobile, which could delay or minimize superparasitism. The three species were significantly capable of discriminating previously parasitized hosts, suggesting that this strategy is commonly present in the guild of fruit fly parasitoids attacking larvae in the Neotropics. Finally, our data also suggest that the studied species have the potential to be considered as suitable biological control agents. However, more studies are necessary to better understand the advantages and limitations that each one presents as natural enemies of fruit fly pests under field conditions.
We thank the technical support provided by Velisario Rivera, César Gálvez, Patricia López and Patricia Rosario. We also thank Javier Valle Mora (ECOSUR) for statistical advice, and the Moscafrut program (SAGARPA-IICA) for providing the biological material for this study. The CONACyT granted a doctoral scholarship to A.A. (CVU 350406).